Front | Back |
What did Archer (1991) find in their meta-analysis?
|
Archer analysed the results of 230 males over 5 studies and found a low positive correlation between testosterone and aggression.
|
What should we note when considering Archer's findings?
|
The type of ppt and the form and measurement of aggression differed substantially between studies.
|
What did Book et al (2001) find in their meta-analysis?
|
A meta-analysis of 45 studies established a mean correlation of 0.14 between testosterone and aggression.
|
What did Archer et al (2005) claim about Book et al's meta-analysis?
|
Archer et al claim that methodological problems with this study meant that a correlation of 0.08 was more appropriate.
|
From when is the male sex hormone testosterone thought to influnce aggression and why?
|
From young adulthood due to its action on brain areas involved in controlling aggression.
|
What did Dabbs et al (1987) find in their study of aggression and testosterone?
|
Dabbs et al measured salivary testosterone in violent and non-violent criminals. Those with the highest testosterone levels had a history of primarily violent crimes, whereas those with the lowest levels had committed only non-violent crimes.
|
What did Lindman et al (1987) find in their study of testosterone and aggression?
|
Lindman et al found that young males who behaved aggressively when drunk had higher testosterone levels than those who did not act aggressive.
|
Explain Wingfield et al's (1990) challenge hypothesis.
|
The challenge hypothesis proposes that in monogamous species, testosterone levels should only rise above the baseline breeding level in response to social challenges such as male-male aggression or threats to status. As the human species ic onsidered to be monogamous, this would predict that male testosterone levels would rise sharply in response to such challenges. In such situations, a testosterone surge is to be expected, with a consequent increase in aggression, provided that the threat is deemed relevant to reproductive competition (eg. a dispute over a female).
|
What effect does cortisol have in terms of aggression? According to whom?
|
Dabbs et al (1991): Cortisol appears to have a mediating effect on other aggression-related hormones, such as testosterone, possibly because it increases anxiety and the likelihood of social withdrawal.
|
What do high levels of cortisol do?
|
High levels of cortisol inhibit testosterone levels and so inhibit aggression.
|
Which studies have reported low levels of cortisol in certain people?
|
Virkkunen: violent offenders
Tennes and Kreye: violent school children |
What do Tennes and Kreye's and Virkkunen's study suggest?
|
Their findings suggest that although relatively high testosterone is the primary biochemical influence on aggression, low cortisol also play an important role by increasing the likelihood of aggressive behaviour.
|
What do Albert et al (1993) claim?
|
Albert et al claim that despite many studies showing a positive correlation between testosterone and aggression, other studies find no such relationship, particularly those that have compared testosterone levels of aggressive and less aggressive individuals.
In addition, most studies showing a positive correlation have involved small samples of men within prisons, using either self-report measures of aggression or judgement based solely on the severity of the crime committed. |
What does Mazur (1985) suggest that may affect the internal validity of aggression studies?
|
Mazur suggests that we should distinguish aggression from dominance. Individuals act aggressively when their intent is to inflict injury, whereas they act dominantly if their wish is to achieve or maintain status over another.
|
What implications does Mazur's claim have for Wingfielk's challenge hypothesis?
|
Mazur claims that aggression is just one form of dominance behaviour. In non-human animals, the influence of testosterone on dominance behaviour might be shown in aggressive behaviour. In humans, however, the influence of testosterone on dominance is likely to be expressed in more varied and subtle ways (eg. through status-striving behaviour).
|